Hippocampal Non-Theta-Contingent Eyeblink Classical Conditioning: A Model System for Neurobiological Dysfunction

Abstract
Typical information processing is thought to depend on the integrity of neurobiological oscillations that may underlie coordination and timing of cells and assemblies within and between structures. The 3–7 Hz bandwidth of hippocampal theta rhythm is associated with cognitive processes essential to learning and depends on the integrity of cholinergic, GABAergic, and glutamatergic forebrain systems. Since several significant psychiatric disorders appear to result from dysfunction of medial temporal lobe (MTL) neurochemical systems, preclinical studies on animal models may be an important step in defining and treating such syndromes. Many studies have shown that the amount of hippocampal theta in the rabbit strongly predicts the acquisition rate of classical eyeblink conditioning and that impairment of this system substantially slows the rate of learning and attainment of asymptotic performance. Our lab has developed a brain–computer interface that makes eyeblink training trials contingent upon the explicit presence or absence of hippocampal theta. The behavioral benefit of theta-contingent training has been demonstrated in both delay and trace forms of the paradigm with a two- to fourfold increase in learning speed over non-theta states. The non-theta behavioral impairment is accompanied by disruption of the amplitude and synchrony of hippocampal local field potentials, multiple-unit excitation, and single-unit response patterns dependent on theta state. Our findings indicate a significant electrophysiological and behavioral impact of the pretrial state of the hippocampus that suggests an important role for this MTL system in associative learning and a significant deleterious impact in the absence of theta. Here, we focus on the impairments in the non-theta state, integrate them into current models of psychiatric disorders, and suggest how improvement in our understanding of neurobiological oscillations is critical for theories and treatment of psychiatric pathology. Recent findings suggest that an estimated 18.1–36.1% of the global population will suffer from a mental disorder, as classified by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, during their lifetime (1). Onset of these conditions can begin as early as childhood or not appear until late adulthood. One of the primary areas affected by mental illness is cognitive functioning, including attention and memory. Cognitive disruption is seen in a wide range of psychiatric disorders, including, but not limited to, major depressive disorder (MDD) (2), schizophrenia (3), and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (4). Due to its efficacy in both humans and animal models, eyeblink conditioning (EBC) has proven valuable as a behavioral marker of cognitive impairment in mental illness. Through studies of human patients and animal models, researchers have identified disruptions in electrophysiological activity in each of these disorders (5–8). This review summarizes a series of findings on the relationship between theta oscillations in the hippocampus and EBC in the rabbit. We propose that EBC, which is remarkably similar behaviorally and neurobiologically in humans, can be a productive model system that can serve as a marker for psychiatric disorders and allow invasive local field potential (LFP) and single-unit analyses to investigate their neural substrates. We have developed a brain–computer interface that allows us to give training trials in the explicit presence (T+) or absence (T−) of theta in the CA1 region of dorsal hippocampus. A major feature of this interface is that, unlike drug, lesion, or genetic manipulations, our method allows the phasic increases and decreases of theta that characterize intact hippocampal function and may be a critical aspect of theta’s influence on cognitive processes. We will show that EBC training in the explicit absence of theta reproduces several important behavioral and electrophysiological dysfunctions similar to what is observed in major psychiatric disorders. We argue that the electrophysiological markers at the cellular level during disordered behavioral performance will aid in our understanding of these pathologies and set the course for manipulations or treatments that can restore function or prevent the progression of disease. A major theme will be that neurobiological oscillations, especially theta, serve as important coordinators and facilitators of distributed cognitive brain systems and that the disintegration of these areas is responsible for cognitive impairment and, in extreme cases, psychiatric disorders. We conclude with recommendations for the directions such research may take. Rabbit classical EBC is a widely used model of associative learning. It has been used in research involving humans (9) and non-human animals to investigate the neural substrates of associative learning (10). The EBC paradigm typically involves the presentation of a relatively neutral conditioned stimulus (CS), such as a tone, paired with, but preceding, the presentation of behaviorally relevant unconditioned stimulus (US), such as a corneal airpuff. After sufficient pairings, the subject learns to perform an adaptive eyeblink conditioned response (CR) to the CS, prior to the arrival of the airpuff US. EBC is most commonly presented in one of two general paradigms, delay or trace conditioning. In delay EBC, the CS and US overlap and coterminate. The essential neural circuitry for delay EBC is well established and is contained within the cerebellum [for review: (11)]. The primary site of plasticity has been localized in the interpositus nucleus (IPN). Lesions of the IPN completely prevent acquisition of CRs and eliminate responding in previously trained animals without preventing eyeblinks to the UR (12). In addition to the IPN, the cerebellar cortex has also been shown to be necessary for delay EBC (13), playing a role in the precise timing and amplitude of the CR. Information about the US projects from the inferior olive (IO) to Purkinje cells in the cerebellar cortex and granule cells of the IPN via climbing fibers. CS-related information projects from the lateral pontine nuclei (LPN) to the cerebellar cortex and IPN through the mossy fiber pathway. This cerebellar pathway is essential for delay EBC acquisition and performance, but there are also structures that seem to play a modulatory role. The hippocampus, a structure strongly implicated in learning and memory, is unnecessary for learning the delay paradigm (14), though electrophysiological studies have shown conditioning-dependent changes in cellular response profiles over training (15, 16). Additionally, lesions of the amygdala have been shown to disrupt reflex facilitation in rabbits (17). Lee and Kim (18) provide evidence that the amygdala and hippocampus modulate the emotional and muscular components of EBC, respectively, interacting to allow for the overall learned behavior. The trace form of EBC alters the paradigm by introducing a stimulus-free period between CS offset and US onset. This form of EBC still requires the cerebellar pathway discussed above (19), but lesion and inactivation studies have shown that it is influenced by the amygdala (20, 21), and requires the medial prefrontal cortex (22) and hippocampus (23). Pharmacological inactivation of the hippocampus with scopolamine, a muscarinic acetylcholine (ACh) receptor antagonist, prevented learning; however, a day of training with saline infusions resulted in a gradual acquisition of the paradigm as if training had just begun (24). Disruption of hippocampal functioning via lesions or pharmacological inactivation of major inputs has also been shown to cause behavioral deficits (25–27). Additionally, electrophysiological studies have identified conditioning-related changes in hippocampal cellular responding during the trace paradigm. Multiple-unit recordings have demonstrated gradual increases in response magnitude during the late half of the trace period as training progresses (28). McEchron and Disterhoft (29, 30) have identified several unique response profiles for hippocampal pyramidal cells at the single-unit level. The response profiles most associated with CR learning show increases in pyramidal cell firing to both the CS and US early in training; however, as the animal approaches behavioral asymptote, the response to the US, but not to the CS, begins to decrease. Additionally, recent work has shown that conditioning-related increases in single-unit firing continue through retrieval of the consolidated memory (31). Eyeblink conditioning does not serve solely as an animal model, having been used in human subjects for over a century (32). As in rabbits, patients with cerebellar damage are impaired in learning the delay and trace forms of EBC (33–35). Those suffering hippocampal damage fail to acquire trace, but are able to learn delay EBC (9, 36–38). Additionally, neuroimaging work has implicated a role for the prefrontal cortex in trace EBC (39, 40). Due to the well-defined circuitry necessary for successful EBC performance, this paradigm is able to provide critical input into the neural regions affected in several psychiatric disorders. Early research in patients with MDD implicated cerebellar dysfunction primarily through neuroimaging studies (41–43). The behavioral effects identified with EBC serve to corroborate regional dysfunction observed in neuroimaging studies. Training patients on both delay and trace EBC, Greer et al. (44) provided behavioral evidence indicating abnormalities in cerebellar processing. They found a significant decrease in the number of CRs in MDD patients compared to controls across both forms. While these results do not allow for differentiation of cerebellar and hippocampal dysfunction, comparison of the delay and trace paradigms has been used in other disorders to differentiate functional regions. Grillon et al. (45) compared performance on both the delay and trace EBC paradigms in patients suffering from panic disorder. There was no difference in performance between patients and control subjects on the delay task; however, patients performed significantly worse on the trace paradigm, showing a delayed acquisition rate. This pattern of results indicates hippocampal dysfunction and potential deficits in declarative memory in panic disorder patients. As panic disorder requires unexpected panic attacks, the authors posit that these deficits may underlie a patient’s inability to identify predictive cues. Results have been less clear in studies of schizophrenia. Early work indicated a possible enhancement of delay EBC, with patients demonstrating faster acquisition rates than controls (46, 47). More recently, several studies have found impaired delay EBC performance through decreased acquisition rates (48–53), decreased CR amplitude (54), and less adaptively timed CRs (50) compared to controls, as well as linking those deficits to decreased cerebellar volume (49) and blood flow (52). Additionally, Marenco et al. (55) demonstrated an increase in short latency (non-adaptively timed) CRs during trace EBC in schizophrenic patients. Eyeblink conditioning has been especially prominent in the study of AD, being used both in animal models and in human patients. Studies have shown deficits in acquisition rate for both the delay (33, 56–58) and trace paradigms (59–61), with a larger effect in the delay paradigm (59). Papka and Woodruff-Pak (62) identified the number of trials necessary to accurately assess delay EBC in AD patients, providing a more efficient test of cognitive performance that may serve as a diagnostic tool in differentiating normal aging from dementia (63). While delay EBC can be acquired normally after hippocampal removal, pharmacological disruption of the septo-hippocampal cholinergic system leads to deficits in performance (26, 64). As cholinergic disruption is a key component of AD pathology (65–67), parallel findings between rabbits with cholinergic dysfunction and AD patients provide validation of the animal model. Furthermore, galantamine, a cholinesterase inhibitor, facilitates EBC performance in aged, but not young, animals, suggesting that it counteracts the decrease in cholinergic activity associated with aging (68). Cholinergic systems have long been associated with cognitive functions, such as attention and memory, that are often affected in psychiatric disorders (69). The basal forebrain cholinergic system is deserving of particular attention due to the target structures of its separate cholinergic neuron populations. The first originates in the horizontal limb of the diagonal band of Broca (DBB) and nucleus basalis and projects to areas of the cortex, such as the mPFC (70), an area involved in sustained attention (71). A separate population of cholinergic projections originates in the medial septum and vertical DBB targeting the dorsal hippocampus, an essential region for encoding of declarative memory. Numerous lines of research have converged to show deficits in cholinergic functions underlying the cognitive deficits of several psychiatric disorders. In AD patients, postmortem studies have indicated a loss of cholinergic neurons in the nucleus basalis (72), a finding supported recently using MRI (73). Additionally, the primary treatments for AD involve acetylcholinesterase inhibitors as a means of increasing cholinergic activity (74–76). Other disorders linked to cholinergic dysfunction include schizophrenia and MDD. In humans, muscarinic antagonists have been shown to increase the severity and duration of both positive and cognitive symptoms in schizophrenic patients (77, 78). Furthermore, anti-muscarinics can lead to a temporary psychosis resembling schizophrenia in healthy subjects (79). Postmortem studies have shown a decrease in muscarinic ACh receptors in schizophrenia patients (80, 81). Additionally, acetylcholinesterase inhibitors have been useful in treating hallucinations (82). These findings have been corroborated in animal models where muscarinic antagonists have led to cognitive impairments and psychosis indicating behaviors in rodent models (78). Though less research has been conducted in MDD patients, recent studies have shown antidepressant effects of scopolamine, a muscarinic receptor antagonist (83), and decreased levels of muscarinic receptors in MDD. As hippocampal theta power is positively correlated with ACh activity (84, 85), it may be possible to use our model system, in which the non-theta group likely shows diminished cholinergic activity immediately preceding conditioning trials, to explore electrophysiological and behavioral bases of these disorders. Neurobiological oscillations have been associated with memory processes, feature binding, and consciousness through their ability to synchronize across and within brain regions, though a definitive function has not been established (86–89). Synchronization of cellular activity within a region can be clearly seen in the strong relationship of single-units and neurobiological oscillations with many cells having preferred phases of the oscillation to increase their firing rates (90–93). Oscillatory potentials can be divided into a several frequency bands based on functional behaviors with which they are associated, as well as cellular and pharmacological mechanisms underlying their generation (88). It is important to note that these different oscillations do not operate in isolation, with multiple theories proposing an interaction between two frequency bands being essential for cognitive processes (89, 94, 95). As normal functioning requires the complex interplay of oscillatory activity across brain regions, lack of synchrony or perturbations of these endogenous signals can lead to detrimental effects associated with several psychiatric disorders. In recent years, research into causes and potential treatments for schizophrenia has increasingly emphasized a basic understanding the neural circuits and processes leading to the myriad of symptoms. Due to the large-scale network believed to be involved in the disorder, abnormalities in oscillatory dynamics seem poised to play a major role in explaining the cognitive deficits (5). At a relatively broad level, schizophrenia has been associated with alterations in the relative power of several oscillatory frequencies associated with cognitive processes, including theta (4–7 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta (15–30 Hz), and gamma (40–100 Hz) (5–8, 96, 97). Some research has also indicated the importance of understanding different frequency oscillations in the context of their cross-frequency modulation, particularly in regard to gamma and theta (97). Researchers have also attempted to examine disruptions in neural dynamics and relate them to specific disruptions of behavioral tasks (6). A common finding in electrophysiological research is phase locking of oscillatory activity following stimulus presentation, a phenomenon typically allowing for coordination of neuronal firing across a distributed system. However, schizophrenic patients have shown delays in phase locking following auditory (98) and visual stimulation (99), with the degree of phase locking correlated with the extent of visual hallucinations and thought disorders (100). Additionally, while increases in frontal midline theta are typically seen following initiation of working memory tasks (101), schizophrenic patients show no increase, and at times a decrease, of evoked theta at various degrees of working memory load (102). These disturbances have been linked to a lack of theta coherence between left frontal and temporal EEG recordings in schizophrenics compared to controls (103). At the cellular level, a loss of synchrony may affect the optimal balance between excitation and inhibition, particularly in regard to activity of GABAergic interneurons (96). Similarly, MDD has been characterized by alterations in oscillatory activity across theta, alpha, and beta bandwidths, but has also shown decreases in delta (0.5–3 Hz) activity (104, 105). These patterns result in changes of the relative ratio of each frequency, creating a highly heterogeneous state (104). MDD patients show a convoluted pattern of effects in terms of oscillatory synchronization. While MDD is characterized by increased synchronization of alpha and beta, as well as frontal theta (105, 106), several studies have also demonstrated a decrease in frontal theta power relative to controls (107–109). Furthermore, increases in theta power following deep brain stimulation have been shown to predict long-term clinical efficacy of treatment (110). Extending beyond frontal theta, animal models of MDD have revealed the effects of theta in the medial temporal lobe (MTL). Zheng and Zhang (111) found a decrease in theta phase coupling between the ventral hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex that was associated with decrease in synaptic plasticity of the pathway. Furthermore, Sauer et al. (112) have shown reduced synchrony of theta and gamma oscillations in the prelimbic cortex attributed in part to a decrease of output from prelimbic GABAergic interneurons. Finally, it is important to consider neurobiological oscillations in AD, a disorder most commonly noted for the presence of amyloid beta (Aβ) plaques. Recent work has shown the potential of oscillatory activity as a means of early AD diagnosis. Compared to controls, AD patients have shown lower theta phase locking to stimuli (8), as well as decreased functional connectivity as measured by phase synchronization (113–115). Utilizing Granger causality and stochastic event synchrony, Dauwels et al. (116) demonstrated that loss of EEG synchrony can accurately predict occurrence of AD based on pre-dementia data. Using EEG synchrony as a screening tool can potentially be improved upon by applying principal component analysis before estimating synchrony (117). Animal models of AD are also being used to characterize the cellular basis of maladaptive alterations in oscillatory and cellular activity. Increasing disruption of hippocampal theta oscillations has been shown in Aβ overproducing transgenic mice as a function of age (118). Guitérrez-Lerma et al. (119) found that the two different types of hippocampal theta are affected differentially by a variety of Aβ peptides. Hippocampal pyramidal cells are disrupted in normal aging, showing a decrease in excitability over time (120, 121), as well as in AD models in which desynchronization of action potential generation leads to a shift in the excitatory/inhibitory equilibrium (122). Hippocampal Aβ also impacts functioning in target structures. For example, investigating a decrease in hippocampal theta power, Villette et al. (123) showed a reduction of firing activity in GABAergic neurons in the medial septum. Importantly, this reduction in firing was not caused by a loss of neurons, but rather an alteration in their normal firing pattern. Our model system permits analysis of specific electrophysiological responses to the conditioning stimuli in terms of LFP synchrony and cellular reactivity with precise control of hippocampal theta state. Though psychiatric disorders are accompanied by disruptions in several frequency bands, work in our lab has focused on the hippocampal theta rhythm (3–12 Hz). Across a range of species and tasks, hippocampal theta has been implicated in spatial (90, 91, 124–126), declarative (127–129), and working (101, 130, 131) memory processes. Within the theta band, Kramis et al. (132) identified two types of theta that are pharmacologically and behaviorally different, cholinergic (3–7 Hz) and non-cholinergic (8–12 Hz) theta. Cholinergic theta is present during alert immobility and is eliminated by the muscarinic ACh receptor antagonist, atropine. Non-cholinergic theta appears during voluntary movements and is unaffected by atropine. Both types of theta have been shown in the rabbit depending on the task (132, 133), with cholinergic theta being the dominant frequency during EBC. In 1978, Berry and Thompson (134) identified a cognitive benefit of hippocampal theta that would serve as the foundation of the future development of our brain–computer interface (BCI). They found a strong positive correlation between pre-training hippocampal theta and learning rate, a finding that was recently replicated in rabbits (135) and extended into human spatial learning (136, 137). Several studies have shown that lesions to the MS reduce hippocampal theta power and significantly slow learning of an EBC task (25, 26, 64, 138). Additionally, eliciting theta through MS stimulation or water deprivation has led to increases in learning rate (139, 140). It is important to note, however, that all of these studies utilized non-physiological alterations to the LFP, disrupting the natural ebb and flow that some believe to underlie the role of theta in cognitive processes (88, 141, 142). Also, it has been shown that artificial stimulation of the MS distorts the normal physiological response patterns of theta-related cells in the hippocampus (143). Thus, allowing the normal fluctuations of theta and non-theta states, as our interface does, may be a key to understanding the natural role of oscillations in behavioral learning and cellular response profiles. To address that important issue, Seager et al. (144) developed a BCI capable of making training trials contingent on fluctuations in the naturally occurring oscillations. For a comprehensive overview of the BCI design and methodology, see Hoffmann et al. (145). Briefly, the BCI uses real-time spectral analysis to restrict EBCC trials to the explicit presence (T+) or absence (T−) of hippocampal theta (Figure 1). To accomplish this, either chronic monopolar electrodes or independently moveable tetrodes are implanted in area CA1 of the hippocampus. During training, a custom LabView program calculates a ratio of power at bandwidths specified by the experimenter. For our work that involves calculating the ratio of theta (3.5–8.5 Hz) to non-theta (0.5–3.5 Hz and 8.5–22 Hz) in real time. The ratio is calculated for 640-ms running time intervals, offset by 160 ms to allow for partially overlapping samples. In the T+ condition, a trial is triggered if the ratio of theta to non-theta exceeds 1.0 for three consecutive intervals. A trial is triggered in the T− condition if the ratio falls below 0.3 for three consecutive intervals. This methodology allows for the different training groups to receive trials under opposite theta conditions while still allowing for the natural fluctuation between trials. Figure 1. Surface plots of power spectral density (PSD) of the pre-CS period triggering a trial for 1 day of training for an animal in the T+ (A) and T− condition (B). Trials in the T+ condition were consistently triggered under conditions of high theta and low delta and alpha. The T− condition was triggered by periods of low theta and high delta or alpha. Note that the T− condition is more heterogeneous than T+, with trials being triggered under both high delta and high alpha conditions. Figures created from data published in Cicchese et al. (146). The initial BCI study examined the effects of theta-contingent training during a delay EBC paradigm (144). Subjects were divided into four groups: (1) trials triggered in the explicit presence of theta (T+); (2) trials in the explicit absence of theta (T−); (3) T+ yoked controls, inter-trial intervals matched to the T+ subjects regardless of theta state; and (4) T− yoked controls. Animals trained under T− conditions learned significantly slower than those in the T+ condition (Figure 2A), requiring more trials to reach asymptotic performance (eight CRs out of nine consecutive trials; 8/9 CRs) and showing a lower percentage of CRs across training. Additionally, T− subjects required significantly more trials to the 8/9 criterion than their yoked controls (Figure 2B), highlighting the detrimental effects of T− training. This is important to note when considering non-theta-contingent training as a natural model of a dysfunctional hippocampus, as these results coincide with the previous findings that pharmacologically disrupting hippocampal functioning is more detrimental to delay EBC than having no hippocampus (64). These findings have been extended to trace EBC in several studies. Utilizing the same four groups (T+, T−, T+ yoked, and T− yoked), Griffin et al. (28) showed that T− animals required significantly more trials to reach early (fifth CR) and late (8/9 CRs) learning criteria, demonstrated a lower percentage of CRs on the first 4 days of training, and required more trials to reach fifth CR than their yoked control counterparts. These results have been replicated by our lab with T− animals reaching the fifth CR criterion later than T+ animals (146, 147) and T− animals showing a lower percentage of CRs across the first 4 days of training (148). Taken together, the deficits seen in both delay and trace EBC mirror the patterns seen in patients and animal models of several psychiatric disorders. This is particularly relevant for disorders in which the cholinergic system is affected, such as AD, as the T− condition reflects a period where the cholinergic system is not engaged. Figure 2. (A) Average number of trials required to reach behavioral criteria in delay (8/9 CRs) and trace (fifth CR and 8/9 CRs) forms of EBC. Animals trained under T− conditions required significantly more trials than T+ animals to reach asymptotic performance (8/9 CRs) in delay conditioning, as well as more trials to reach early (fifth CR) and asymptotic (8/9 CRs) behavioral markers. (B) Average difference in the number of trials to reach behavioral criteria from yoked controls. T− animals needed more trials than controls to reach asymptotic performance of delay conditioning and more trials to reach the early learning criteria of trace conditioning. The differences from yoked controls provide evidence of detrimental performance in the T− condition, showing that T−/T+ differences are not simply an effect of improved performance in the T+ condition. *p < 0.05. Delay figures adjusted from Seager et al. (144), trace figures adjusted from Griffin et al. (28). Furthermore, our BCI findings point to a potential treatment for cognitive deficits seen in aging and AD. Asaka et al. (149) examined the effects of theta-contingent training on aged animals, those that typically show learning deficits (150, 151). Four groups of animals were trained, young T+, young yoked controls, aged T+, and aged yoked controls. As expected, aged yoked controls performed significantly worse than young yoked controls, taking longer to reach several late learning behavioral criteria (including 8/9 CRs and 80% CRs in a session). However, aged T+ animals learned significantly faster than aged yoked controls, and showed no difference in learning rate from young yoked controls (Figure 3). Importantly, the benefit of T+ training persisted past behavioral indicators of asymptotic performance in aged animals, suggesting that sustained accurate performance, a cerebellar-dependent function, is also affected by oscillatory state. While aging is accompanied by a decrease in cholinergic activity, the presence of 3–7 theta in the hippocampus demonstrates that periods of relatively normal cholinergic activity persist that can be engaged as a non-pharmacological intervention for cognitive deficits. Figure 3. Average number of trials to reach the late learning criteria (8/9 CRs and 80% CRs) for young yoked controls, aged T+ triggered, and aged yoked control animals. Aged yoked controls required more trials to reach both criteria than young yoked controls, indicating disrupted performance in aged animals. Aged animals trained under T+ conditions performed better than their yoked control counterparts and showed no difference from the young yoked controls. Thus, theta-contingent training alleviated the cognitive deficits seen in aged controls. *p < 0.05. Figure adapted from data published in Asaka et al. (149). These behavioral results are consistent with recent studies in human subjects. Using magnetoencephalographic (MEG) recordings, Guderian et al. (152) found a positive correlation between pretrial theta amplitude in the MTL and recall rate in an episodic learning task. Following this demonstration, Fell et al. (153) recorded bilaterally along the longitudinal axis of the MTL with intracranial EEG. Enhancement of hippocampal theta predicted successful encoding of a word recognition task. Similarly, Lega et al. (154) recording from the hippocampus of neurosurgical patients showed higher theta power during encoding. Interestingly, the researchers identified a slow and fast center in the theta rhythm, and only the slow theta (~3 Hz) showed this pattern. In addition to deleterious behavioral effects, training in the explicit absence of theta has been shown to have negative effects on hippocampal electrophysiology at the LFP, multiple-unit, and single-unit levels. Previous work in rats has demonstrated a phase reset of the local theta rhythm following stimulus presentation (155, 156). Using the trace EBC paradigm, our lab has replicated this phase reset and shown coherent rhythmicity at theta frequencies in T+ animals following both CS and US presentation (147, 148); however, animals trained under T− conditions display a delayed onset of phase reset, as well as decreased rhythmicity in theta frequency compared to T+ animals. These results in the T− condition are important to consider as McCartney et al. (156) have shown that the phase reset produced by relevant stimuli provides ideal conditions for LTP to occur, suggesting a decrease in neural plasticity when trained in the absence of theta. Additionally, this delayed phase reset is comparable to that seen in schizophrenic patients in response to both auditory (98) and visual (99) stimuli. Coinciding with the effects on LFPs, T− training impairs both the magnitude and rhythmicity of hippocampal multiple-units. During trace EBC, multiple-units in T− animals inhibited below baseline firing during presentation of the tone and through the 500-ms trace interval, while those in T+ animals showed excitation (28). Note that this indicates an active suppression or inhibition of unit firing under T− conditions rather than simply the absence of an excitatory response. While this effect was seen on the second and third days of training, Darling et al. (147) linked this decrease in activity of T− units to behavioral criterion, showing significant inhibition at the early (fifth CR) and late (8/9 CRs) learning markers. Furthermore, similar to what has been seen in LFPs, T− multiple-units lack rhythmicity in firing during the trace interval, whereas T+ units fired coherently at 6.25 Hz (147). Early work in rabbit EBC showed that conditioning-dependent changes in multiple-unit activity were the result of changes in pyramidal cell activity (16, 157). To replicate this, our theta-triggered work was continued with single-unit recordings of hippocampal pyramidal cells. To determine whether changes in multiple-unit activity were caused by large firing rate changes in a few critical cells or by a change in the overall number of cells responding in a particular way (firing rate increasing or decreasing), Cicchese et al. (146) analyzed pyramidal cell responses by their qualitative (rate increasing or decreasing) and quantitative (response magnitude) properties. Early in learning, putative pyramidal cells were more likely to decrease their firing rate during the tone period in T− than in T+ animals and more likely to increase their firing rate during both the tone and trace periods in T+ compared to T− (Figure 4). Importantly, there were no theta-contingent differences in the magnitude of either firing rate increases or decreases. These findings suggest that the role of theta in cellular firing is related to the recruitment of additional units firing a particular pattern, rather than a drastic change in rate of relatively few cells. This implies that an optimal hippocampal ensemble response for EBC consists of more widespread excitation of pyramidal cells rather than a sparse code of heightened responses by a few cells. Thus, theta may serve to optimize the ratio of cells showing excitation or inhibition, leading to a dysfunctional balance in the absence of theta. This conclusion would agree with findings from models of schizophrenia (96) and AD (122), implicating a shift in the excitatory/inhibitory equilibrium as a potential cellular mechanism. Additionally, Rutishauser et al. (158) found a positive correlation between performance of a memory task and coordination of hippocampal spike timing to the local theta rhythm. This is consistent with our results showing a learning deficit in T− subjects accompanied with less coherence of pyramidal cell response direction. Figure 4. Surface plots showing the standard scores (10-ms bins) of all rate decreasing (A) and rate increasing (B) cells averaged across the entire training session (truncated to 4 for illustration purposes). Note that rate decreases and increases during tone presentation and are sustained past airpuff presentation. (C) A greater percentage of cells in the T− condition were rate decreasing during the tone than in the T+ condition. (D) Cells in the T+ condition were more likely to increase their firing during the tone and trace periods than those in T−. *p < 0.025. Figure adapted from data published in Cicchese et al. (146). Due to the distributed memory system involved in trace EBC, it is important to consider how non-theta-contingent training may negatively affect processing in other necessary regions. LFP recordings taken from hippocampal CA1, and cerebellar IPN and HVI, have revealed striking theta-contingent differences in both rhythmicity and synchronization between areas that may underlie dysfunctional processing during training (148). Coinciding with improved behavioral performance, T+ animals showed theta rhythmicity time-locked to conditioning stimuli in the cerebellum and precise theta antiphase (180o) synchronization between CA1 and IPN/HVI LFPs. By contrast, T− performance deficits were accompanied by an absence of theta oscillations in IPN and HVI, as well as a lack of synchronization with CA1. These results are consistent with human studies showing an increase in theta synchrony across distributed regions following induction of MTL theta oscillations (159), as well as with fear conditioning studies in rats showing a synchronized theta activity between the lateral amygdala and hippocampus following training (160). The lack of synchronization across areas is of particular interest in light of psychiatric research. Animal models of MDD have implicated the absence of ventral hippocampus–mPFC theta phase coupling with decreased synaptic plasticity (111), while a loss of cortical EEG synchrony is a fundamental feature in AD (113–116). These oscillatory disruptions likely cause a decline in functional connectivity, failing to coordinate activity across regions necessary for cognitive processes. The hippocampus does not directly project to the cerebellum, but may have an indirect influence through its effects on the mPFC. The mPFC is necessary for trace EBC (161, 162) and projects to the lateral pontine nucleus, which conveys important CS-related mossy fiber input to the cerebellum (163). Previous work has identified a mPFC cellular response profile characterized by inhibition followed by a period of persistent excitation in response to tone presentation (164). This pattern is thought to increase the salience of the tone by increasing the signal-to-noise ratio. Darling et al. (147) capitalized on our theta-triggered paradigm by recording simultaneously from area CA1 and the mPFC (caudal anterior cingulate region) under T+ and T− conditions. Interestingly, though the inhibitory/excitatory pattern was replicated in T+ animals, it was absent in those trained under T− conditions. This finding implies that mPFC processing is highly related to hippocampal theta state and that our T− animals may fail to apply proper motivational salience to the conditioning stimuli. Importantly, the increased theta synchrony between hippocampus and amygdala during Pavlovian conditioning (160) raises the possibility that motivational and emotional input from the basolateral amygdala normally converges on the mPFC in synchrony with hippocampal input to modulate salience; thus, in the absence of hippocampal theta, a lack of converging input disrupts processing of the stimuli. A similar effect is seen in schizophrenia where patients show maladaptive motivational salience when rating reinforcements (165) and when learning to discriminate between a predictive CS+ and neutral CS− (166, 167). Additionally, compared to controls, schizophrenia patients show increased neural activity to the CS− in regions associated with learning (166, 167). Thus, our T− condition appears to replicate some important findings from the human literature and relate them to neuronal response patterns in important structures. As the study of cognitive processes has moved away from discrete functional regions to distributed neural networks (168), it is essential to understand the oscillatory activity capable of synchronizing these anatomically disparate regions (88, 141, 142). Similarly, a focus on electrophysiological disruption in psychiatric disorders is proving invaluable as loss of synchronization across regions is a common feature underlying their pathology (8, 112–114). Using our BCI, we have shown that training in the explicit absence of hippocampal theta produces deficits in EBC expected of a number of psychiatric conditions. Furthermore, these behavioral deficits are accompanied by electrophysiological disruptions at the LFP (147, 148), multiple- (28, 147), and single-unit (146) levels that are characteristic of conditions as disparate as schizophrenia, MDD, and AD. Of particular interest are the patterns seen across the regions necessary for EBC, with a lack of synchrony between hippocampus and cerebellum (148) and the absence of relevant response patterns in mPFC units (147). Though our non-theta-triggering has proven effective at modeling the electrophysiological correlates of a disrupted system, it is important to note that it still has room to grow. The BCI allows for trials to be delivered in the presence of a specific brain state, but does not give control of that activity. Thus, fluctuations in pretrial activity that may typically be abnormal in disorders cannot be controlled for. However, the ability of our non-theta-triggering to model interruption of distributed neural networks without lesions or pharmacological intervention provides a tool for studying psychiatric disorders in a more natural way, allowing for decreased levels of the given frequency, as is typical in illness, rather than complete abolition. An important challenge to our findings has recently been published in the form of a failure to replicate the benefits of theta-contingent EBC (169). The authors found that animals trained under T− conditions were more likely to acquire the paradigm than yoked controls or those trained in the presence of theta; however, it should be noted that T− animals required more sessions to reach behavioral criterion than their yoked controls, consistent with our findings. These findings seem to contradict numerous studies in animals (28, 135, 144, 146–148) and humans (152–154), showing beneficial learning effects of increased hippocampal/MTL pretrial theta. Due to a fundamental methodological difference, it is possible that the study by Nokia and Wikgren (169) does not directly apply to our work. Specifically, in their study, all subjects were presented with a full session of unpaired conditioning before training began. This introduces latent inhibition as a major confound to later learning effects. While T+ and T− animals each received the unpaired session, work has not been completed to investigate how effects of latent inhibition may interact with theta-contingent learning conducted after unpaired presentations. For example, unpaired presentations of CS and US have been shown to cause a baseline EEG shift from pre- to post-exposure (170), and latent inhibition produces significantly reduced hippocampal unit responsiveness to a tone CS (171). An effect of the unpaired session is suggested by the unusually low percentage of animals that successfully acquired the CS–US association. Additionally, T+ animals that reached criterion took an average of ~5 fewer sessions than their yoked control counterparts; however, that difference was not significant, likely due to insufficient power (T+: n = 4, yoked control: n = 2; 0.05 < p < 0.10). While these results highlight the complex relationship between oscillatory potentials and different learning paradigms, potential differences in hippocampal functioning caused by latent inhibition, as well as low statistical power, prevent a direct comparison to our theta and non-theta-contingent findings. Knowing the established effect of theta on cognitive processes, it will be critical to further study its role. In particular, further exploration of mPFC theta activity could serve to bridge the gap between animal and human recording studies. Much of the theta work in human subjects has centered on frontal midline theta, but it is still unclear what the neural correlates underlying these oscillations are (101). By understanding the relationship between oscillations in subcortical structures and those recorded by scalp EEG, it would be possible to utilize neurofeedback training as a possible treatment for psychiatric conditions, similar to what has been done in patients with ADHD (172, 173). Though our BCI does not allow for direct manipulations of theta, new research methods, such as optogenetics, may make this possible. Using optogenetic stimulation of the medial septum could provide precise temporal control of theta rhythm induction. During this stimulation, simultaneous recordings from relevant areas (hippocampus, mPFC, and cerebellum) could provide further insight into the electrophysiological relationship of the distributed network. Specifically, this methodology would allow for precise control over theta phase during stimulation presentation. Considering the prominent model of separate encoding and retrieval phases of theta (128), our T+ group could be further studied by looking at trials triggered consistently on either the peak or through of theta. It is possible that triggering during the retrieval phase of the theta rhythm could be equally detrimental to training in the absence of theta, an idea recently supported using theta-contingent training in conjunction with threshold values to target specific phases (174). Furthermore, optogenetic manipulation of theta state could be used in conjunction with conditional genetic knockout animal models to identify potential benefits of inducing synchronous neural activity in animals that are typically lacking. Initial studies into this possibility could utilize classical conditioning to allow for discrete learning points. By doing so, optogentic stimulation of the medial septum at theta frequency could be initiated prior to CS delivery, ensuring synchronous and homogeneous neural activity when learning is expected to occur. Dependent on the results, additional work should be completed to examine the amount of time asynchronous activity must be disrupted for alleviation of behavioral deficits. While research has shown physiological difference in cellular responding to naturally occurring and artificially stimulated theta (143), it is likely that optogentically induced theta would still provide benefits in animals with genetically disrupted theta oscillations. Several studies using the Morris water maze support this notion. Deficits in performance caused by disruption of hippocampal theta via pharmacological inactivation of the medial septum (175–177) or fimbria-fornix lesions (178) were overcome by artificial stimulation at theta frequency. Conversely, recent contextual fear conditioning work found a decrease in performance as a result of artificial theta stimulation (179). The authors propose, however, that the continuous stimulation provided at a fixed frequency may have interrupted the normal oscillatory processes of the rat; specifically, the constant theta likely interfered with the natural theta entrainment experienced during walking and sniffing as the rat explores its environment. Furthermore, they suggest that stimulation coinciding with an external cue, such as a tone CS, may show enhancement in performance similar to the aforementioned studies. Although our work has focused on the theta to non-theta [3.5–8 Hz/(0.5–3.5 Hz + 8.5–22 Hz)] ratio, the LabView program can be set with any frequency range in the numerator and denominator. With this flexibility, future studies could utilize the BCI for training contingent on different frequency bands and exploration of different definitions of non-theta. Our non-theta state is heterogeneous, with major contributions of delta (0.5–2 Hz) and alpha (8–12 Hz) compared to the homogeneous theta band. This heterogeneity may underlie the detrimental effects seen in our non-theta conditioning. It will be important for future studies to alter the frequencies defined as non-theta, including using individual frequency bands in the denominator, to determine whether the decrease in theta or the heterogeneity of oscillatory bands is responsible for adverse learning. In work by others, triggering trials based on sharp-wave ripple oscillations (150–250 Hz) has been shown to increase EBC learning rate and increase the phase locking of theta oscillations to conditioning stimuli (180), suggesting that the heterogeneity of our non-theta state plays an important role. Therefore, it will be important to continue research into the effects of ripple-contingent training and their relation to theta. As discussed previously, several frequency bands are disrupted in psychiatric disorders. In light of the differences in behavioral and neurochemical characteristics of these various oscillations, it is critical to understand the contributions of each to cognitive processes and psychiatric pathology. Multidisciplinary approaches as discussed above will be an important contributor to this effort. All authors listed, have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication. The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. This material is based on work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant Nos. IOB-0517575 and IOS-1121969 (SB). 1. Kessler RC, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, Chatteri S, Lee S, Ormel J, et al. The global burden of mental disorders: an update from the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. Epidemiol Psychiatr Soc (2009) 18:23–33. doi:10.1017/S1121189X00001421 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 2. Hammar Å, Årdal G. Cognitive functioning in major depression – a summary. Front Hum Neurosci (2009) 3:26. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.026.2009 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 3. Gold JM. Cognitive deficits as treatment targets in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2004) 72:21–8. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.008 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 4. Ferri CP, Prince M, Brayne C, Brodaty H, Fratiglioni L, Ganguli M, et al. Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. Lancet (2005) 366:2112–7. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67889-0 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 5. Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron (2006) 52:155–68. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.020 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 6. Uhlhaas PJ, Haenschel C, Nikolić D, Singer W. The role of oscillations and synchrony in cortical networks and their putative relevance for the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (2008) 34:927–43. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn062 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 7. Başar E. Brain oscillations in neuropsychiatric disease. Dialogues Clin Neurosci (2013) 15:291–300. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 8. Başar E, Schmiedt-Fehr C, Mathes B, Femir B, Emek-Savaş DD, Tülay E, et al. What does the broken brain say to the neuroscientist? Oscillations and connectivity in schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s disease, and bipolar disorder. Int J Psychophysiol (2015). doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.02.004 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 9. Clark RE, Squire LR. Classical conditioning and brain systems: the role of awareness. Science (1998) 280:77–81. doi:10.1126/science.280.5360.77 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 10. Gormezano I, Schneiderman N, Deaux E, Fuentes I. Nictitating membrane: classical conditioning and extinction in the albino rabbit. Science (1962) 138:33–4. doi:10.1126/science.138.3536.33 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 11. Christian KM, Thompson RF. Neural substrates of eyeblink conditioning: acquisition and retention. Learn Mem (2003) 10:427–55. doi:10.1101/lm.59603 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 12. McCormick DA, Clark GA, Lavond DG, Thompson RF. Initial localization of the memory trace for a basic form of learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1982) 79:2731–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.79.8.2731 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 13. Freeman JH, Steinmetz AB. Neural circuitry and plasticity mechanisms underlying delay eyeblink conditioning. Learn Mem (2011) 18:666–77. doi:10.1101/lm.2023011 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 14. Schmaltz LW, Theios J. Acquisition and extinction of a classically conditioned response in hippocampectomized rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). J Compar Physiol Psychol (1972) 79:328–33. doi:10.1037/h0032531 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 15. Berger TW, Alger B, Thompson RF. Neuronal substrate of classical conditioning in the hippocampus. Science (1976) 192:483–5. doi:10.1126/science.1257783 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 16. Berger TW, Thompson RF. Identification of pyramidal cells as the critical elements in hippocampal neuronal plasticity during learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1978) 75:1572–6. doi:10.1073/pnas.75.3.1572 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 17. Weisz DJ, Harden DG, Xiang Z. Effects of amygdala lesions on reflex facilitations and conditioned response acquisition during nictitating membrane response conditioning in rabbit. Behav Neurosci (1992) 106(2):262–73. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.106.2.262 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 18. Lee T, Kim JJ. Differential effects of cerebellar, amygdalar, and hippocampal lesions on classical eyeblink conditioning in rats. J Neurosci (2004) 24:3242–50. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5382-03.2004 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 19. Woodruff-Pak DS, Lavond DG, Thompson RF. Trace conditioning: abolished by cerebellar nuclear lesions but not lateral cerebellar cortex aspirations. Brain Res (1985) 348:249–60. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(85)90443-3 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 20. Ng KH, Freeman JF. Amygdala inactivation impairs eyeblink conditioning in developing rats. Dev Psychobiol (2014) 56:999–1007. doi:10.1002/dev.21180 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 21. Siegel JJ, Taylor W, Gray R, Kalmbach B, Zemelman BV, Desai NS, et al. Trace eyeblink conditioning in mice is dependent upon the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, cerebellum, and amygdala: behavioral characterization and functional circuitry. eNeuro (2015) 2(4):1–29. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0051-14.2015 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 22. Oswald B, Knuckley B, Mahan K, Sanders C, Powell DA. Prefrontal control of trace versus delay eyeblink conditioning: role of the unconditioned stimulus in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Behav Neurosci (2006) 120:1033–42. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.08.013 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 23. Solomon PR, Vander Schaaf ER, Thompson RF, Weisz DJ. Hippocampus and trace conditioning of the rabbit’s classically conditioned nictitating membrane response. Behav Neurosci (1986) 100:729–44. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.100.5.729 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 24. Kaneko T, Thompson RF. Disruption of trace conditioning of the nictitating membrane response in rabbits by central cholinergic blockade. Psychopharmacology (1997) 131:161–6. doi:10.1007/s002130050279 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 25. Berry SD, Thompson RF. Medial septal lesions retard classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane response in rabbits. Science (1979) 205:209–11. doi:10.1126/science.451595 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 26. Salvatierra AT, Berry SD. Scopolamine disruption of septo-hippocampal activity and classical conditioning. Behav Neurosci (1989) 103:715–21. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.103.4.715 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 27. Allen MT, Padilla Y, Gluck MA. Ibotenic acid lesions of the medial septum retard delay eyeblink conditioning in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Behav Neurosci (2002) 116:733–8. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.116.4.733 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 28. Griffin AL, Asaka Y, Darling RD, Berry SD. Theta-contingent trial presentation accelerates learning rate and enhances hippocampal plasticity during trace eyeblink conditioning. Behav Neurosci (2004) 118:403–11. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.118.2.403 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 29. McEchron MD, Disterhoft JF. Sequence of single neuron changes in CA1 hippocampus of rabbits during acquisition of trace eyeblink conditioned responses. J Neurophysiol (1997) 78:1030–44. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 30. McEchron MD, Disterhoft JF. Hippocampal encoding of non-spatial trace conditioning. Hippocampus (1999) 9:385–96. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:43.3.CO;2-B PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 31. Hattori S, Chen L, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF. Robust hippocampal responsivity during retrieval of consolidated associative memory. Hippocampus (2015) 25(5):655–69. doi:10.1002/hipo.22401 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 32. Gormezano I. Bibliography of human eyeblink conditioning (1899-1985). In: Woodruff-Pak DS, Steinmetz JE, editors. Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Volume 1: Applications in Humans. Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers (2000). p. 275–307. Google Scholar 33. Solomon PR, Stowe GT, Pendlebury WW. Disrupted eyelid conditioning in a patient with damage to cerebellar afferents. Behav Neurosci (1989) 103:898–902. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.103.4.898 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 34. Woodruff-Pak DS, Papka M, Ivry RB. Cerebellar involvement in eyeblink classical conditioning in humans. Neuropsycholology (1996) 10:443–58. doi:10.1037//0894-4105.10.4.443 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 35. Woodruff-Pak DS. Aging and classical conditioning: parallel studies in rabbits and humans. Neurobiol Aging (1988) 9:511–22. doi:10.1016/S0197-4580(88)80108-8 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 36. Daum I, Channon S, Canavan AG. Classical conditioning in patients with severe memory problems. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (1989) 52:47–51. doi:10.1136/jnnp.52.1.47 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 37. Gabrieli J, McGlinchey-Berroth R, Carrillo M, Gluck M, Cermak L, Disterhoft JF. Intact delay-eyeblink classical conditioning in amnesia. Behav Neurosci (1995) 109:819–27. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.109.5.819 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 38. McGlinchey-Berroth R, Carrillo MC, Gabrieli JD, Brawn CM, Disterhoft JF. Impaired trace eyeblink conditioning in bilateral, medial-temporal lobe amnesia. Behav Neurosci (1997) 111:873–82. doi:10.1037//0735-7044.111.5.873 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 39. Blaxton TA, Zeffiro TA, Gabrieli JDE, Bookheimer SY, Carrillo MC, Theodore WH, et al. Functional mapping of human learning: a positron emission tomography activation study of eyeblink conditioning. J Neurosci (1996) 16:4032–40. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 40. Schreurs B, McIntosh A, Bahro M, Herscovitch P, Sunderland T, Molchan S. Lateralization and behavioral correlation of changes in regional cerebral blood flow with classical conditioning of the human eyeblink response. J Neurophysiol (1997) 77:2153–63. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 41. George MS, Ketter TA, Post RM. SPECT and PET imaging in mood disorders. J Clin Psychiatry (1993) 54:6–13. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 42. Soares JC, Mann JJ. The anatomy of mood disorders – review of structural neuroimaging studies. Biol Psychiatry (1997) 41:86–106. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00006-6 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 43. Escalona PR, Early B, McDonald WM, Doraiswamy PM, Shah SA, Husain MM, et al. Reduction of cerebellar volume in major depression: a controlled MRI study. Depression (1993) 1:156–8. doi:10.1002/depr.3050010307 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 44. Greer TL, Trivedi MH, Thompson LT. Impaired delay and trace eyeblink conditioning performance in major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord (2005) 86:235–45. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2005.02.006 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 45. Grillon C, Lissek S, McDowell D, Levenson J, Pine DS. Reduction of trace but not delay eyeblink conditioning in panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry (2007) 164:283–9. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.164.2.283 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 46. Spain B. Eyelid conditioning and arousal in schizophrenic and normal subjects. J Abnorm Psychol (1966) 71:260–6. doi:10.1037/h0023596 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 47. Sears LL, Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS. Cerebellar functional abnormalities in schizophrenia are suggested by classical eyeblink conditioning. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 48:204–9. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00247-X PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 48. Brown SM, Kieffaber PD, Carroll CA, Vohs JL, Tracy JA, Shekhar A, et al. Eyeblink conditioning deficits indicate timing and cerebellar abnormalities in schizophrenia. Brain Cognit (2005) 58:94–108. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.011 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 49. Edwards CR, Newman S, Bismark A, Skosnik PD, O’Donnell BF, Shekhar A, et al. Cerebellum volume and eyeblink conditioning in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res (2008) 162:185–94. doi:10.1016/j.psychresns.2007.06.001 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 50. Bolbecker AR, Mehta CS, Edwards CR, Steinmetz JE, O’Donnell BF, Hetrick WP. Eye-blink conditioning deficits indicate temporal processing abnormalities in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2009) 111:182–91. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2009.03.016 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 51. Bolbecker AR, Steinmetz AB, Mehta CS, Forsyth JK, Klaunig MJ, Lazar EK, et al. Exploration of cerebellar-dependent associative learning in schizophrenia: effects of varying and shifting interstimulus interval on eyeblink conditioning. Behav Neurosci (2011) 125:687–98. doi:10.1037/a0025150 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 52. Parker KL, Andreasen NC, Liu D, Freeman JH, O’Leary DS. Eyeblink conditioning in unmedicated schizophrenia patients: a positron emission tomography study. Neuroimaging (2013) 214:402–9. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.07.006 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 53. Coesmans M, RÖder CH, Smit AE, Koekkoek SKE, De Zeeuw CI, Frens MA, et al. Cerebellar motor learning deficits in medicated and medication-free men with recent-onset schizophrenia. J Psychiatry Neurosci (2014) 39:3–11. doi:10.1503/jpn.120205 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 54. Forsyth JK, Bolbecker AR, Mehta CS, Klaunig MJ, Steinmetz JE, O’Donnell BF, et al. Cerebellar-dependent eyeblink conditioning deficits in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Bull (2012) 38:751–9. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbq148 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 55. Marenco S, Weinberger DR, Schreurs BG. Single-cue delay and trace classical conditioning in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2003) 53:390–402. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01506-8 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 56. Solomon PR, Levine E, Bein T, Pendlebury WW. Disruption of classical conditioning in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging (1991) 12:283–7. doi:10.1016/0197-4580(91)90004-4 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 57. Woodruff-Pak DS, Thompson RF. Classical conditioning of the eyeblink response in the delay paradigm in adults aged 18-83 years. Psychol Aging (1988) 3:219–29. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.3.3.219 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 58. Ferrante LS, Woodruff-Pak DS. Longitudinal investigation of eyeblink classical conditioning in elderly human subjects. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci (1995) 50:42–50. doi:10.1093/geronb/50B.1.P42 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 59. Woodruff-Pak DS, Papka M. Alzheimer’s disease and eyeblink conditioning: 750 ms trace vs. 400 ms delay paradigm. Neurobiol Aging (1996) 17:397–404. doi:10.1016/0197-4580(96)00022-X CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 60. Kishimoto Y, Oku I, Nishigawa A, Nishimoto A, Kirino Y. Impaired long-trace eyeblink conditioning in a Tg2576 mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett (2012) 506:155–9. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2011.10.071 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 61. Kishimoto Y, Kirino Y. Presenilin 2 mutation accelerates the onset of impairment in trace eyeblink conditioning in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease overexpressing human mutant amyloid precursor protein. Neurosci Lett (2013) 538:15–9. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2013.01.025 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 62. Papka M, Woodruff-Pak DS. Number of trials needed to assess human eyeblink classical conditioning. Psychol Aging (1996) 11:373–6. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.11.2.373 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 63. Woodruff-Pak DS. Eyeblink classical conditioning differentiates normal aging from Alzheimer’s disease. Integr Physiol Behav Sci (2001) 36:87–108. doi:10.1007/BF02734044 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 64. Solomon PR, Solomon SD, Vander Schaaf E, Perry HE. Altered activity in the hippocampus is more detrimental to classical conditioning than removing the structure. Science (1983) 220:329–31. doi:10.1126/science.6836277 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 65. Bierer L, Haroutunian V, Gabriel S, Knott P, Carlin L, Purohit DP, et al. Neurochemical correlates of dementia severity in Alzherimer’s disease: relative importance of the cholinergic deficits. J Neurochem (1995) 64:749–60. doi:10.1046/j.1471-4159.1995.64020749.x CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 66. Collie A, Maruff P. The neuropsychology of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2000) 24:365–74. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00012-9 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 67. Popp J, Arlt S. Pharmacological treatment of dementia and mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2011) 24:556–61. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e32834b7b96 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 68. Weible AP, Oh MM, Lee G, Disterhoft JF. Galantamine facilitates acquisition of hippocampus-dependent trace eyeblink conditioning in aged rabbits. Learn Mem (2004) 11:108–15. doi:10.1101/lm.69804 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 69. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Central cholinergic systems and cognition. Ann Rev Psychol (1997) 48:649–84. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.649 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 70. Mesulam M, Mufson EJ, Levey AI, Wainer BH. Cholinergic innervation of cortex by the basal forebrain: cytochemistry and cortical connections of the septal area, diagonal band nuclei, nucleus basalis (substantia innominata and hypothalamus) in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol (1983) 214:170–97. doi:10.1002/cne.902140206 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 71. Kahn JB, Ward RD, Kahn LW, Rudy NM, Kandel ER, Balsam PD, et al. Medial prefrontal lesions in mice impair sustained attention but spare maintenance of information in working memory. Learn Mem (2012) 19:513–7. doi:10.1101/lm.026302.112 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 72. Whitehouse PJ, Price DL, Clark AW, Coyle JT, DeLong MR. Alzheimer disease: evidence for selective loss of cholinergic neurons in the nucleus basalis. Ann Neurol (1981) 10:122–6. doi:10.1002/ana.410100203 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 73. Muth K, SchÖnmeyer R, Matura S, Haenschel C, SchrÖder J, Pantel J. Mild cognitive impairment in the elderly is associated with volume loss of the cholinergic basal forebrain region. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 67:588–91. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.026 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 74. Jelic V, Kivipelto M, Winblad B. Clinical trials in mild cognitive impairment: lessons for the future. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2006) 77:429–38. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2005.07926 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 75. Raschetti R, Albanese E, Vanacore N, Maggini M. Cholinesterase inhibitors in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review of randomized trials. PLoS Med (2007) 4:e338. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0040338 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 76. Karakaya R, Fuber F, SchrÖder J, Pantel J. Pharmacological treatment of mild cognitive impairment as a prodromal syndrome of Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Neuropharmacol (2013) 11:102–8. doi:10.2174/1570159113804999487 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 77. Tandon R. Antipsychotics in the treatment of schizophrenia: an overview. J Clin Psychiatry (2011) 1:4–8. doi:10.4088/JCP.10075su1.01 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 78. Carruthers SP, Gurvich CT, Rossell SL. The muscarinic system, cognition and schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2015) 55:393–402. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.05.011 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 79. Yeomans JS. Role of tegmental cholinergic neurons in dopaminergic activation, antimuscarinic psychosis and schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology (1995) 12:3–16. doi:10.1016/0893-133X(94)00054-4 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 80. Mancama D, Arranz MJ, Landau S, Kerwin R. Reduced expression of the muscarinic 1 receptor cortical subtype in schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet (2003) 119B:2–6. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.20020 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 81. Newell KA, Zavitsanou K, Jew SK, Huang XF. Alterations of muscarinic and GABA receptor binding in the posterior cingulate cortex in schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2007) 31:225–33. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.07.004 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 82. Patel SS, Attard A, Jacobsen P, Shergill S. Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (AChEI’s) for the treatment of visual hallucinations in schizophrenia: a review of the literature. BMC Psychiatry (2010) 10:69. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-10-69 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 83. Furey ML, Drevets WC. Antidepressant efficacy of the antimuscarinic drug scopolamine: a randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2006) 63:1121–9. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.10.1121 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 84. Marrosu F, Portas C, Mascia MS, Casu MA, Fá M, Giagheddu M, et al. Microdialysis measurement of cortical and hippocampal acetylcholine release during sleep-wake cycle in freely moving cats. Brain Res (1995) 671:329–32. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(94)01399-3 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 85. Monmaur P, Collet A, Puma C, Frankel-Kohn L, Sharif A. Relations between acetylcholine release and electrophysiological characteristics of theta rhythm: a microdialysis study in the urethane-anesthetized rat hippocampus. Brain Res Bull (1997) 42:141–6. doi:10.1016/S0361-9230(96)00200-6 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 86. Singer W. Neuronal synchrony: a versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron (1999) 24:49–65. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80821-1 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 87. Buzsáki G. Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron (2002) 33:325–40. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00586-x PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 88. Buzsáki G. Rhythms of the Brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, Inc (2006). Google Scholar 89. Lisman J. The theta/gamma discrete phase code occurring during the hippocampal phase precession may be a more general brain coding scheme. Hippocampus (2005) 15:913–22. doi:10.1002/hipo.20121 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 90. O’Keefe J, Recce ML. Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus (1993) 3:317–30. doi:10.1002/hipo.450030307 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 91. Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL, Wilson MA, Barnes CA. Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences. Hippocampus (1996) 6:149–72. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:23.0.CO;2-K PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 92. Klausberger T, Magill PJ, Márton LF, Roberts JD, Cobden PM, Buzsáki G, et al. Brain-state- and cell-type-specific firing of hippocampal interneurons in vivo. Nature (2003) 441:844–8. doi:10.1038/nature01374 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 93. Klausberger T, Somogyi P. Neuronal diversity and temporal dynamics: the unity of hippocampal circuit operations. Science (2008) 321:53–7. doi:10.1126/science.1149381 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 94. Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS, Kirsch HE, et al. High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science (2006) 313:1626–8. doi:10.1126/science.1128115 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 95. Belluscio MA, Mizuseki K, Schmidt R, Kempter R, Buzsáki G. Cross-frequency phase-phase coupling between θ and γ oscillations in the hippocampus. J Neurosci (2012) 32:423–35. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4122-11.2012 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 96. Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Oscillations and neuronal dynamics in schizophrenia: the search for basic symptoms and translational opportunities. Biol Psychiatry (2015) 77(12):1001–9. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.11.019 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 97. Kirihara K, Rissling AJ, Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Light GA. Hierarchical organization of gamma and theta oscillatory dynamics in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2012) 71:873–80. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.016 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 98. Kwon JS, O’Donnell BF, Wallenstein GV, Greene RW, Hirayasu Y, Nestor PG, et al. Gamma frequency-range abnormalities to auditory stimulation in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1999) 56:1001–5. doi:10.1001/archpsych.56.11.1001 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 99. Spencer KM, Nestor PG, Niznikiewicz MA, Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. Abnormal neural synchrony in schizophrenia. J Neurosci (2003) 23:7407–11. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 100. Spencer KM, Nestor PG, Perlmutter R, Niznikiewicz MA, Klump MC, Frumin M, et al. Neural synchrony indexes disordered perception and cognition in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2004) 101:17288–93. doi:10.1073/pnas.0406074101 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 101. Hsieh L, Ranganath C. Frontal midline theta oscillations during working memory maintenance and episodic encoding and retrieval. Neuroimage (2014) 85:721–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.201.08.003 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 102. Schmiedt C, Brand A, Hildebrandt H, Basar-Eroglu C. Event-related theta oscillations during working memory tasks in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res (2005) 25:936–47. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.015 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 103. Ford JM, Mathalon DH, Whitfield S, Faustman WO, Roth WT. Reduced communication between frontal and temporal lobes during talking in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2002) 51:485–92. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)0135-X PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 104. Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA, Rytsälä H, Suominen K, Isometsä E, KähkÖnen S. Composition of brain oscillations in ongoing EEG during major depression disorder. Neurosci Res (2006) 56:133–44. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2006.06.006 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 105. Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA. Altered structure of dynamic electroencephalogram oscillatory pattern in major depression. Biol Psychiatry (2014) 77(12):1050–60. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.011 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 106. Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA, Rytsälä H, Suominen K, Isometsä E, KähkÖnen S. Impaired functional connectivity at EEG alpha and theta frequency bands in major depression. Hum Brain Mapp (2007) 28:247–61. doi:10.1002/hbm.20275 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 107. Mulert C, Juckel G, Brunnmeier M, Karch S, Leicht G, Mergl R, et al. Rostral anterior cingulate cortex activity in the theta band predicts responses to antidepressive medication. Clin EEG Neurosci (2007) 38:78–81. doi:10.1177/155005940703800209 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 108. Saletu B, Anderer P, Saletu-Zyhlarz GM. EEG topography and tomography (LORETA) in diagnosis and pharmacotherapy of depression. Clin EEG Neurosci (2010) 41:203–10. doi:10.1177/155005941004100407 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 109. Smart O, Tiruvadi V, Mayberg H. Multimodal approaches to define network oscillations in depression. Biol Psychiatry (2015) 77(12):1061–70. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.01.002 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 110. Broadway JM, Holtzheimer PE, Hilimire MR, Parks NA, Devylder JE, Mayberg HS, et al. Frontal theta cordance predicts 6-month antidepressant response to subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression: a pilot study. Neuropsychopharmacology (2012) 37:1764–72. doi:10.1038/npp.2012.23 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 111. Zheng C, Zhang T. Synaptic plasticity-related neural oscillations on hippocampus-prefrontal cortex pathway in depression. Neuroscience (2015) 292:170–80. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.01.071 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 112. Sauer J, Strüber M, Bartos M. Impaired fast-spiking interneuron function in a genetic mouse model of depression. eLife (2015) 4:979. doi:10.7554/eLife.04979 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 113. Koenig T, Prichep L, Dierks T, Hubl D, Wahlund LO, John ER, et al. Decreased EEG synchronization in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging (2005) 26:165–71. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.03.008 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 114. Park YM, Che HJ, Im CH, Jung HT, Bae SM, Lee SH. Decreased EEG synchronization and its correlation with symptom severity in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Res (2008) 62:112–7. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2008.06.009 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 115. van Straaten EC, Scheltens P, Gouw AA, Stam CJ. Eyes-closed task-free electroencephalography in clinical trials for Alzheimer’s disase: an emerging method based upon brain dynamics. Alzheimers Res Ther (2014) 6:86. doi:10.1186/s13195-014-0086-x CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 116. Dauwels J, Vialatte F, Latchoumane C, Jeong J, Cichocki A. EEG synchrony analysis for early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: a study with several synchrony measures and EEG data sets. IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc (2009) 2009:2224–7. doi:10.1109/IEMBS.2009.5334862 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 117. Al-Jumeily D, Iram S, Vialette F, Fergus P, Hussain A. A novel method of early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease based on EEG signals. Sci World J (2015) 2015:931387. doi:10.1155/2015/931387 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 118. Scott L, Feng J, Kiss T, Needle E, Atchison K, Kawabe TT, et al. Age-dependent disruption in hippocampal theta oscillation in amyloid-β overproducing transgenic mice. Neurobiol Aging (2012) 33:.e13–1481. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.010 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 119. Guitérrez-Lerma AI, Ordaz B, Peña-Ortega F. Amyloid beta peptides differentially affect hippocampal theta rhythms in vitro. Int J Pept (2013) 2013:328140. doi:10.1155/2013/328140 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 120. Moyer JR, Power JM, Thompson LT, Disterhoft JR. Increased excitability of aged rabbit CA1 neurons after trace eyeblink conditioning. J Neurosci (2000) 20:5476–82. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 121. Wu WW, Oh M, Disterhoft JF. Age-related biophysical alterations of hippocampal pyramidal neurons: implications for learning and memory. Ageing Res Rev (2002) 1:181–207. doi:10.1016/S1568-1637(01)00009-5 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 122. Kurudenkandy FR, Zilberter M, Biverstål H, Presto J, Honcharenko D, StrÖmberg R, et al. Amyloid-β-induced action potential desynchronization and degradation of hippocampal gamma oscillations is prevented by interference with peptide conformation change and aggregation. J Neurosci (2014) 34:11416–25. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1195-14.2014 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 123. Villette V, Poindessous-Jazat F, Simon A, Léna C, Roullot E, Bellessort B, et al. Decreased rhythmic GABAergic septal activity and memory-associated θ oscillations after hippocampal amyloid-β pathology in the rat. Neurobiol Dis (2010) 30:10991–1003. doi:10.1523/JNEUOSCI.6284-09.2010 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 124. Kahana MJ, Sekuler R, Caplan JB, Kirschen M, Madsen JR. Human theta oscillations exhibit task dependence during virtual maze navigation. Nature (1999) 399:781–4. doi:10.1038/21645 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 125. Vertes RP. Hippocampal theta rhythm: a tag for short-term memory. Hippocampus (2005) 15:923–35. doi:10.1002/hipo.20118 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 126. Burgess N, O’Keefe J. Models of place and grid cell firing and theta rhythmicity. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2011) 21:734–44. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2011.07.002 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 127. Klimesch W, Doppelmayr M, Russegger H, Pachinger T. Theta band power in the human scalp EEG and the encoding of new information. Neuroreport (1996) 7:1235–40. doi:10.1097/00001756-199605170-00002 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 128. Hasselmo ME. What is the function of hippocampal theta rhythm? – Linking behavioral data to phasic properties of field potential and unit recording data. Hippocampus (2005) 15:936–49. doi:10.1002/hipo.20116 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 129. Osipova D, Takashima A, Oostenveld R, Fernández G, Maris E, Jensen O. Theta and gamma oscillations predict encoding and retrieval of declarative memory. J Neurosci (2006) 26:7523–31. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1948-06.2006 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 130. Hwang G, Jacobs J, Geller A, Danker J, Sekuler R, Kahana MJ. EEG correlates of verbal and nonverbal working memory. Behav Brain Funct (2005) 1:20. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-1-20 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 131. Jones MW, Wilson MA. Phase precession of medial prefrontal cortical activity relative to the hippocampal theta rhythm. Hippocampus (2005) 15:867–73. doi:10.1002/hipo.20119 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 132. Kramis R, Vanderwolf CH, Bland BH. Two types of hippocampal rhythmical slow activity in both the rabbit and the rat: relations to behavior and effects of atropine, diethyl ether, urethane, and pentobarbital. Exp Neurol (1975) 49:58–85. doi:10.1016/0014-4886(75)90195-8 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 133. Sinclair BR, Seto MG, Bland BH. Theta-cells in CA1 and dentate layers of hippocampal formation: relations to slow-wave activity and motor behavior in the freely moving rabbit. J Neurophysiol (1982) 48:1214–25. Google Scholar 134. Berry SD, Thompson RF. Prediction of learning rate from the hippocampal electroencephalogram. Science (1978) 200:1298–300. doi:10.1126/science.663612 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 135. Nokia M, Penttonen M, Korhonen T, Wikgren J. Hippocampal theta (3-8Hz) activity during classical eyeblink conditioning in rabbits. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2008) 90:62–70. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2008.01.005 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 136. Caplan JB, Madsen JR, Raghavachari S, Kahana MJ. Distinct patterns of brain oscillations underlie two basic parameters of human maze learning. J Neurophysiol (2001) 86:368–80. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 137. Caplan JB, Madsen JR, Schulze-Bonhage A, Aschenbrenner-Scheibe R, Newman EL, Kahana MJ. Human theta oscillations related to sensorimotor integration and spatial learning. J Neurosci (2003) 23:4726–36. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 138. Solomon PR, Gottfried KE. The septohippocampal cholinergic system and classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. J Comp Physiol Psychol (1981) 95:322–30. doi:10.1037/h0077779 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 139. Deupree D, Coppock W, Willer H. Pretraining septal driving of hippocampal rhythmic slow activity facilitates acquisition of visual discrimination. J Comp Physiol Psychol (1982) 96:557–62. doi:10.1037/h0077908 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 140. Berry SD, Swain RA. Water deprivation optimizes hippocampal activity and facilitates nictitating membrane conditioning. Behav Neurosci (1989) 103:71–6. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.103.1.71 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 141. Berry SD, Hoffmann LC. Hippocampal theta-dependent eyeblink classical conditioning: coordination of a distributed learning system. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2011) 95:185–9. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2010.11.014 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 142. Hoffmann LC, Cicchese JJ, Berry SD. Harnessing the power of theta: natural manipulations of cognitive performance during hippocampal theta-contingent eyeblink conditioning. Front Syst Neurosci (2015) 9:50. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2015.00050 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 143. Scarlett D, Dypvik AT, Bland BH. Comparison of spontaneous and septally driven hippocampal theta field and theta-related cellular activity. Hippocampus (2004) 14:99–106. doi:10.1002/hipo.10151 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 144. Seager MA, Johnson LD, Chabot ES, Asaka Y, Berry SD. Oscillatory brain states and learning: impact of hippocampal theta-contingent training. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2002) 99:1616–20. doi:10.1073/pnas.032662099 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 145. Hoffmann LC, Cicchese JJ, Berry SD. Hippocampal theta-based brain-computer interface. In: Hassanien AE, Azar AT, editors. Brain-Computer Interfaces. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing (2015). p. 155–84. Google Scholar 146. Cicchese JJ, Darling RD, Berry SD. Pretrial hippocampal theat-state differentiates single-unit response profiles during rabbit trace eyeblink conditioning. Learn Mem (2015) 22:318–22. doi:10.1101/lm.038216.115 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 147. Darling RD, Takatsuki K, Griffin AL, Berry SD. Eyeblink conditioning contingent on hippocampal theta enhances hippocampal and medial prefrontal responses. J Neurophysiol (2011) 105:2213–24. doi:10.1152/jn.00801.2010 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 148. Hoffmann LC, Berry SD. Cerebellar theta oscillations are synchronized by hippocampal theta-contingent trace conditioning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2009) 106:21371–6. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908403106 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 149. Asaka Y, Mauldin KN, Griffin AL, Seager MA, Shurell E, Berry SD. Nonpharmacological amelioration of age-related learning deficits: the impact of hippocampal theta-triggered training. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2005) 102:13284–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0506515102 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 150. Solomon PR, Graves CA. Classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane response in aged rabbits. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1985) 444:486–8. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb37619.x PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 151. Thompson LT, Deyo RA, Disterhoft JF. Nimodipine enhances spontaneous activity of hippocampal pyramidal neurons in aging rabbits at a dose that facilitates associative learning. Brain Res (1990) 535:119–30. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(90)91830-A PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 152. Guderian S, Schott BH, Richardson-Klavehn A, Düzel E. Medial temporal theta state before an event predicts episodic encoding success in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2009) 106:5365–70. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900289106 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 153. Fell J, Ludowig E, Staresina BP, Wagner T, Kranz T, Elger CE, et al. Medial temporal theta/alpha power enhancement precedes successful memory encoding: evidence based on intracranial EEG. J Neurosci (2011) 31:5392–7. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3668-10.2011 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 154. Lega BC, Jacobs J, Kahana M. Human hippocampal theta oscillations and the formation of episodic memories. Hippocampus (2012) 22:748–61. doi:10.1002/hipo.20937 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 155. Givens B. Stimulus-evoked resetting of the dentate theta rhythm: relation to working memory. Neuroreport (1996) 8:159–63. doi:10.1097/00001756-199612200-00032 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 156. McCartney H, Johnson AD, Weil ZM, Givens B. Theta reset produces optimal conditions for long-term potentiation. Hippocampus (2004) 14:684–7. doi:10.1002/hipo.20019 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 157. Berger TW, Rinaldi PC, Weisz DJ, Thompson RF. Single-unit analysis of different hippocampal cell types during classical conditioning of rabbit nictitating membrane response. J Neurophys (1983) 50:1197–219. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 158. Rutishauser U, Ross IB, Mamelak AN, Schuman EM. Human memory strength is predicted by theta-frequency phase-locking of single neurons. Nature (2010) 464:903–7. doi:10.1038/nature08860 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 159. Guderian S, Düzel E. Induced theta oscillations mediate large-scale synchrony with mediotemporal areas during recollection in humans. Hippocampus (2005) 15:901–12. doi:10.1002/hipo.20125 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 160. Seidenbecher T, Laxmi TR, Stork O, Pape HC. Amygdalar and hippocampal theta rhythm synchronization during fear memory retrieval. Science (2003) 301:846–50. doi:10.1126/science.1085818 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 161. Weible AP, McEchron MD, Disterhoft JF. Cortical involvement in acquisition and extinction of trace eyeblink conditioning. Behav Neurosci (2000) 114:1058–67. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.114.6.1058 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 162. Kalmbach BE, Ohyama T, Kredier JC, Riusech F, Mauk MD. Interactions between prefrontal cortex and cerebellum revealed by trace eyelid conditioning. Learn Mem (2009) 16:86–95. doi:10.1101/lm.1178309 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 163. Dailey ME, Buchanan J, Bergles DE, Smith SJ. Mossy fiber growth and synaptogenesis in rat hippocampal slices in vitro. J Neurosci (1994) 14:1060–78. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 164. Weible AP, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF. Activity of single neurons in caudal anterior cingulate cortex during trace eyeblink conditioning in the rabbit. J Neurophysiol (2003) 90:599–612. doi:10.1152/jn.01097.2002 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 165. Roiser JP, Stephan KE, den Ouden HE, Barnes TR, Friston KJ, Joyce EM. Do patients with schizophrenia exhibit aberrant salience? Psychol Med (2009) 39:199–209. doi:10.1017/S0033291708003863 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 166. Jensen J, Willeit M, Zipursky RB, Savina I, Smith AJ, Menon M, et al. The formation of abnormal associations in schizophrenia: neural and behavioral evidence. Neuropsychopharmacology (2008) 33:473–9. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301437 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 167. Diaconescu AO, Jensen J, Wang H, Willeit M, Menon M, Kapur S, et al. Aberrant effective connectivity in schizophrenia patients during appetitive conditioning. Front Hum Neurosci (2011) 4:239. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00239 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 168. Bressler SL, Menon V. Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci (2010) 14:277–90. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 169. Nokia MS, Wikgren J. Effects of hippocampal state-contingent trial presentation on hippocampus-dependent nonspatial classical conditioning and extinction. J Neurosci (2014) 34:6003–10. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4859-13.2014 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 170. Berry SD, Seager MA, Asaka Y, Borgnis RL. Motivational issues in aversive and appetitive conditioning paradigms. In: Woodruff-Pak DS, Steinmetz JE, editors. Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Vol. 2: Animal Models. Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers (2000). p. 287–312. Google Scholar 171. Best MR, Best PJ. The effects of state of consciousness and latent inhibition on hippocampal unit activity in the rat during conditioning. Exp Neurol (1976) 51:564–73. doi:10.1016/0014-4886(76)90180-1 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 172. Bink M, van Nieuwenhuizen C, Popma A, Bongers IL, van Boxtel GJ. Neurocognitive effects of neurofeedback in adolescents with ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Psychiatry (2014) 75:535–42. doi:10.4088/JCP.13m08590 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 173. Holtmann M, Pniewski B, Wachtlin D, Wörz S, Strehl U. Neurofeedback in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a controlled multicenter study of a non-pharmacological treatment approach. BMC Pediatr (2014) 14:202. doi:10.1186/1471-2431-14-202 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 174. Nokia MS, Waselius T, Mikkonen JE, Wikgren J, Penttonen M. Phase matters: responding to and learning about peripheral stimuli depends on hippocampal theta phase at stimulus onset. Learn Mem (2015) 22:307–17. doi:10.1101/lm.038166.115 CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 175. Allen CN, Crawford IL. GABAergic agents in the medial septal nucleus affect hippocampal theta rhythm and acetylcholine utilization. Brain Res (1984) 322:261–7. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(84)90116-1 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 176. Mizumori SJ, Perez GM, Alvarado MC, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Reversible inactivation of the medial septum differentially affects two forms of learning in rats. Brain Res (1990) 528:12–20. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(90)90188-H PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 177. McNaughton N, Ruan M, Woodnorth MA. Restoring theta-like rhythmicity in rats restores initial learning in the Morris water maze. Hippocampus (2006) 16:1102–10. doi:10.1002/hipo.20235 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 178. Turnbull J, Jiang F, Racine R. Hippocampal stimulation of fornical-lesioned rats improves working memory. Can J Neurol Sci (1994) 21:100–3. PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar 179. Lipponen A, Woldemichale BT, Gurevicius K, Tanila H. Artificial theta stimulation impairs encoding of contextual fear memory. PLoS One (2012) 7(11):e48506. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0048506 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar 180. Nokia MS, Penttonen M, Wikgren J. Hippocampal ripple-contingent training accelerates trace eyeblink conditioning and retards extinction in rabbits. J Neurosci (2010) 30:11486–92. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2165-10.2010 PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar Keywords: hippocampus, neurobiological oscillations, theta rhythm, brain–computer interface, cognitive dysfunction, psychiatric disorders Citation: Cicchese JJ and Berry SD (2016) Hippocampal Non-Theta-Contingent Eyeblink Classical Conditioning: A Model System for Neurobiological Dysfunction. Front. Psychiatry 7:1. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00001 Received: 02 July 2015; Accepted: 01 January 2016; Published: 12 February 2016 Edited by: Reviewed by: Copyright: © 2016 Cicchese and Berry. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. *Correspondence: Stephen D. Berry, berrysd@miamioh.edu